Skip to content

Lesson 3: Let’s Try an Experiment!

Lesson 3: Let’s Try an Experiment!

Objective:

Students will explore the importance of randomized assignment in experiments. They will understand that without random assignment, there might be confounding variables and will be able to suggest possible confounding variables.

Materials:

  1. Measuring Tape

Essential Concepts:

Essential Concepts:

Randomized assignment is required to determine cause-and-effect.

Lesson:

  1. Inform students that they will be exploring the question “Why do we need randomized assignment?” by conducting an experiment. Tell students that you have a treatment that can make people taller. Explain that the class will be divided into two groups, one group will get the treatment, and one group will not. The group that does not receive the treatment will be the control group. After the treatment, they will measure the groups to see which is taller. Now divide the class into two groups by placing the taller students in the treatment group and the shorter students in the control group.

    Remember that in an experiment we typically have a treatment group and a control group. Some experiments do not require a before and after as long as the two groups being compared are similar (many medical studies use this method for experiments). In this case, we will run the experiment and then compare average height of the treatment group to the control group.

  2. Tell them that after the treatment group takes the treatment, your statistic to compare groups will be to measure the heights. If the treatment group is taller, then the treatment must have worked. There are two possible outcomes to dividing the class this way:

    1. The students will protest (as they should) and you can start a discussion as to why this is not a good way to divide the class.

    2. OR the students don’t protest and you continue with the experiment. The treatment should be something silly, like waving a ruler in front of the person’s face or by asking them to chant “grow, grow, grow!” three times. After treatment, measure the heights of each group and ask them if they think this is good evidence (do not say “proves”) that the treatment is effective.

  3. Regardless of the outcome, students should recognize that by putting the taller students in one group, the outcome was pre-determined, since they were taller to begin with. This is an example of a confounding factor. Confounding factors are variables that provide an alternative explanation of the effect of the treatment on the outcome variable.

  4. Ask students: “How should students be put into groups?”

  5. Discuss various other methods of grouping students. Students should be able to recognize that you shouldn’t use any characteristics to decide the groups.

  6. Continue discussion of other ways to decide the groups. Use the following questions as a guide:

    1. What about flipping a coin?

    2. What will the height balance look like? Answer: Each group should have about the same balance as the class, though not exactly.

    3. Why is it important that the groups be similar? Answer: Because otherwise, something else might be the cause of the response changing.

  7. Inform students that today the class will begin to design their own experiment using what they have learned over the last few lessons. The question they will investigate is:

    How does our perception of time change when exposed to a stimulus?

  8. They will be trying to determine the length of one minute without the use of time-aids. In their experiment, they will subject some students to a stimulus and others to no stimulus. They will then analyze the data to determine if subjecting students to a stimulus affects the perception of how long a minute of time lasts.

  9. In their DS journals, ask students to answer the following questions about the elements of their experiment:

    1. What is the research question we’re interested in addressing?

    2. Who are the subjects that will be participating in the experiment?

    3. How should we randomly assign the subjects into treatment and control groups? (See step 12 below for an RStudio method that the teacher can use)

    4. What is the outcome variable that we will be measuring? What unit of measurement should we use?

    Note: Students will decide on a treatment to apply to each group on the following day.

  10. As a class, discuss the responses to the questions above (step #9, a-d) and come to a consensus for each question’s answer.

  11. Inform the class that they will be using the answers they have agreed upon as the final design of the class’s experiment.

  12. At the end of the class, the students should be assigned to the treatment or control groups using the randomization method they chose as a class in step #9c from above.

    Note: One method to determine group assignment would be to use the class roster and the sample() function in RStudio. The students have a number that corresponds to their placement on the roster (i.e. student 1’s last name most likely starts with an A, and then we move alphabetically through the roster). You can then use RStudio to randomly select which half of the numbers/students will be assigned to the treatment group.

    > sample(1:30, size = 15, replace = FALSE)

  13. Students will conduct the experiment in the next lesson.

Class Scribes:

One team of students will give a brief talk to discuss what they think the 3 most important topics of the day were.